参考文献

字数:1352

Alem, S., Perry, C. J., Zhu, X., Loukola, O. J., Ingraham, T., Søvik, E., and Chittka,L. (2016). Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. PLoS Biology, 14(10), e1002564.

Alvarez, J. A., and Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes:A meta analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16(1), 17–42.

Amundson, R. (1989). The trials and tribulations of selectionist explanations.In K. Hahlweg and C. A. Hooker (eds.), Issues in Evolutionary Epistemology.Albany, NY: State University of New York Press, 413–432.

Anderson, M. L. (2008). Circuit sharing and the implementation of intelligent systems. Connection Science, 20(4), 239–251.

Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(4), 245–266.

Anderson, M. L. (2016). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain (précis). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e120.

Anderson, M. L., and Finlay, B. L. (2014). Allocating structure to function: The strong links between neuroplasticity and natural selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 918.

Anisfeld, M. (1979). Interpreting “imitative” responses in early infancy. Science, 205, 214–215.

Anisfeld, M. (2005). No compelling evidence to dispute Piaget’s timetable of the development of representational imitation in infancy. In S. Hurley and N. Chater (eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 107–131.

Apperly, I. (2010). Mindreaders: The Cognitive Basis of “Theory of Mind.”. Hove,UK: Psychology Press.

Apps, M. A., Lesage, E., and Ramnani, N. (2015). Vicarious reinforcement learning signals when instructing others. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7),2904–2913.

Aquinas, T. (1272). Summa Theologica (new ed., 2015). Roccasecca, Italy: Xist Publishing.

Atran, S. (2001). The trouble with memes. Human Nature, 12(4), 351–381.

Baer, D. M., and Sherman, J. A. (1964). Reinforcement control of generalized imitation in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1(1),37–49.

Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Bang, D., Roepstorf, A., Rees, G., and Frith, C. (2012). To gether, slowly but surely: The role of social interaction and feedback on the buildup of benefit in collective decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(1), 3–8.

Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., and He, Z. (2010). False belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 110–118.

Bandura, A., Ross, D., and Ross, S. A. (1963). A comparative test of the status envy,social power, and secondary reinforcement theories of identificatory learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(6), 527–534.

Bardi, L.,Regolin,L.,and Simion,F.(2011).Biological motion preference in humans at birth: Role of dynamic and configural properties. Developmental Science, 14 (2), 353–359.

Bardi, L., Regolin, L., and Simion, F. (2014). The first time ever I saw your feet:Inversion efect in newborns’ sensitivity to biological motion. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 986–993.

Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.) (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evo lutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baron Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Barrett, L. F. (2017). How Emotions are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain. Boston,MA: Houghton Miflin Harcourt.

Barton, R. A., and Venditti, C. (2013). Human frontal lobes are not relatively large. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(22),9001–9006.

Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar.In L. Gleitman and E. Wanner (eds.), Language Acquisition: The State of the Art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism and the competition model.In B. MacWhinney and E. Bates (eds.), The Cross-Linguistic Study of Sentence Processing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Battesti, M., Pasquaretta, C., Moreno, C., Teseo, S., Joly, D., Klensch, E., . . . and Mery, F. (2015). Ecology of information: Social transmission dynamics within groups of non social insects. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,Series B: Biological Sciences, 282(1801), 20142480.

Behrens, T. E., Hunt, L. T., Woolrich, M. W., and Rushworth, M. F. (2008). Associative learning of social value. Nature, 456(7219), 245–249.

Behrens, T. E., Woolrich, M. W., Walton, M. E., and Rushworth, M. F. (2007).Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1214–1221.

Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., Futch, S. E., Smith, J. D., Evans, T. A., and Parrish,A. E. (2015). Go when you know: Chimpanzees’ confidence movements reflect their responses in a computerized memory task. Cognition, 142,236–246.

Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., and Perdue, B. M. (2013). Language trained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) name what they have seen but look first at what they have not seen. Psychological Science, 24(5), 660–666.

Berwick, R. C., and Chomsky, N. (2015). Why Only Us: Language and Evolution.Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Bird, G., and Heyes, C. (2005). Efector dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(2), 262–275.

Birdsong, D., and Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence for maturational constraints in second language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(2),235–249.

Blackmore, S. (2000). The Meme Machine. Oxford: Oxford Paperbacks.

Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuro science, 9(4), 267–277.

Blakemore, S. J., Winston, J., and Frith, U. (2004). Social cognitive neuroscience:Where are we heading? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(5), 216–222.

Blass, E. M., Ganchrow, J. R., and Steiner, J. E. (1984). Classical conditioning in newborn humans 2–48 hours of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(2),223–235.

Bloom, P. (2000). How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. Cambridge, MA:The MIT Press.

Bloom, P. (2001). How children learn the meanings of words (précis). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(6), 1095–1103.

Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviourism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals. CUP Archive.

Bock, J., Poeggel, G., Gruss, M., Wingenfeld, K., and Braun, K. (2014). Infant cognitive training preshapes learning relevant prefrontal circuits for adult learning: Learning induced tagging of dendritic spines. Cerebral Cortex,24(11), 2920–2930.

Boeckx, C. (2006). Linguistic Minimalism: Origins, Concepts, Methods, and Aims.Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Bohannon, J. N., MacWhinney, B., and Snow, C. (1990). No negative evidencere visited: Beyond learnability or who has to prove what to whom? Develop mental Psychology, 26(2), 221–226.

Bohannon, J. N., and Stanowicz, L. B. (1988). The issue of negative evidence:Adult responses to children’s language errors. Developmental Psychology, 24(5), 684–689.

Boogert, N. J., Giraldeau, L A., and Lefebvre, L. (2008). Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Animal Behaviour, 76,1735–1741.

Bornkessel Schlesewsky, I., Schlesewsky, M., Small, S. L., and Rauschecker,J. P. (2015). Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition:Common computational properties. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3),142–150.

Bouchard, J., Goodyer, W., and Lefebvre, L. (2007). Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons. Animal Cognition, 10, 259–266.

Bouchard, T. J. (2014). Genes, evolution and intelligence. Behavior Genetics, 44(6), 549–577.

Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1988). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Brem, S., Bucher, K., Halder, P., Summers, P., Dietrich, T., Martin, E., and Brandeis,D. (2006). Evidence for developmental changes in the visual word processing network beyond adolescence. Neuro Image, 29(3), 822–837.

Brown, R., and Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech. Cognition and the Development of Language. New York:Wiley.

Brown, R. L. (2014). Identifying behavioral novelty. Biological Theory, 9(2),135–148.

Brusse, C. (2017). Making do without selection—review essay of “Cultural Evolution: Conceptual Challenges” by Tim Lewens. Biology and Philosophy, 32(2), 307–319.

Bull, R., Phillips, L. H., and Conway, C. A. (2008). The role of control functions in mentalizing: Dual task studies of theory of mind and executive function.Cognition, 107(2), 663–672.

Bur kart,J.M.,Hrdy,S.B.,and Van Schaik,C.P.(2009).Cooperativebreedingand human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Re views, 18 (5), 175–186.

Burnham, D., Kitamura, C., and Vollmer Conna, U. (2002). What’s new, pussycat?On talking to babies and animals. Science, 296(5572), 1435–1435.

Burrow, C. (1993). Epic Romance: Homer to Milton. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Butterfill, S. A., and Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind and Language, 28(5), 606–637.

Butterfill, S., Apperly, I., Rakoczy, H., Spaulding, S., and Zawidzki, T. (2013). Sym posium on S. Butterfill and I. Apperly, “How to Construct a Minimal Theory of Mind.” Mind and Language Symposia at the Brains Blog. http://philo sophyofbrains.com/2013/11/11/symposium on butter fill and apperlys how to construct a minimal theory of mind mind language 28 5 606-63.aspx

Byrne, R. W., and Rapaport, L. G. (2011). What are we learning from teaching?Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 1207–1211.

Caldwell, C. A., Atkinson, M., and Renner, E. (2016). Experimental approaches to studying cumulative cultural evolution. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(3), 191–195.

Calvo Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P.(2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.

Calvo Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P.(2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16(19), 1905–1910.

Campbell, D. T. (1965). Variation and selective retention in socio cultural evolution. Social Change in Developing Areas, 19, 26–27.

Campbell, D. T. (1974). Evolutionary epistemology. In P.A. Schlipp (ed.), The Philosophy of Karl Popper. LaSalle, IL: Open Court, 413–463.

Cardoso, C., Ellenbogen, M. A., and Linnen, A. M. (2014). The efect of intranasal oxytocin on perceiving and understanding emotion on the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT). Emotion, 1, 43–50.

Carey, S. (2009). The Origin of Concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Caro, T. M., and Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals?The Quarterly Review of Biology, 67(2), 151–174.

Carpendale, J. I., and Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind:The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79–96.

Carpenter, M.,and Call,J.(2013).How joint is the joint attention of apes and human infants? In J. Metcalfe and H.S. Terrace (eds.), Agency and Joint Attention . New York: Oxford University Press, 49–61.

Caselli, L., and Chelazzi, L. (2011). Does the macaque monkey provide a good model for studying human executive control? A comparative behavioral study of task switching. PLoS One, 6(6), e21489.

Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., and Eickhof, S. B. (2010). ALE meta analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuro Image, 50(3),1148–1167.

Catmur, C., Mars, R. B., Rushworth, M. F., and Heyes, C. (2011). Making mirrors:Premotor cortex stimulation enhances mirror and counter mirror motor facilitation. Journal of Cognitive Neuro science, 23(9), 2352–2362.

Catmur, C., Press, C., and Heyes, C. (2016). Mirror associations. In R. A. Murphy and R. C. Honey (eds.), The Wiley Handbook of the Cognitive Neuroscience of Learning. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Catmur, C., Santiesteban, I., Conway, J. R., Heyes, C., and Bird, G. (2016). Ava tars and arrows in the brain. Neuro Image, 132, 8–10.

Catmur, C., Walsh, V., and Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology, 17(17), 1527–1531.

Catmur, C., Walsh, V., and Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: The role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2369–2380.

Cavalli Sforza, L. L. (1997). Genes, peoples, and languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94, 7719–7724.

Cavalli Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Chang, E. F., and Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Environmental noise retards auditory cortical development. Science, 300(5618), 498–502.

Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., Ye, H., and Shimojo, S. (2006). The structures of letters and symbols throughout human history are selected to match those found in objects in natural scenes. The American Naturalist, 167(5),E117–E139.

Chater, N., and Christiansen, M. H. (2010). Language acquisition meets language evolution. Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1131–1157.

Chater, N. and Heyes, C. (1994). Animal concepts: Content and discontent. Mind and Language, 9(3), 209–246.

Chater, N., Reali, F., and Christiansen, M. H. (2009). Restrictions on biological adaptation in language evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(4), 1015–1020.

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton and Co.

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and Mind. New York: Foris.

Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. New York: Foris.

Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures (Vol. 16). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Chomsky, N. (1995). Categories and transformations. In The Minimalist Pro gram, 219–394.

Christiansen, M. H., and Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 489–509.

Christiansen, M. H., and Chater, N. (2016). Creating Language: Integrating Evolution, Acquisition, and Processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Christiansen, M. H., and MacDonald, M. C. (2009). A usage based approach to recursion in sentence processing. Language Learning, 59(s1), 126–161.

Churchland, P. S., and Winkielman, P. (2012). Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: How does it work? What does it mean? Hormones & Behavior, 61,392–399.

Cieri, R. L., Churchill, S. E., Franciscus, R. G., Tan, J., Hare, B., Athreya,S., . . . and Wrangham, R. (2014). Craniofacial feminization, social tolerance, and the origins of behavioral modernity. Current Anthropology, 55(4), 419–443.

Clarke, E., and Heyes, C. (2017). The swashbuckling anthropologist: Henrich on the secret of our success. Biology and Philosophy, 32, 289–305.

Collins, A., and Koechlin, E. (2012). Reasoning, learning, and creativity:Frontal lobe function and human decision making. PLoS Biology, 10(3),e1001293.

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., and Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psycho logical Review, 108(1), 204–256.

Cook, J. L. (2014). Task relevance dependent gradients in medial prefrontal and temporoparietal cortices suggest solutions to paradoxes concerning self/other control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42C, 298–302. Cook, M., Mineka, S., Wolkenstein, B., and Laitsch, K. (1985). Observational conditioning of snake fear in unrelated rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 355–372.

Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., and Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons:From origin to function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(2), 177–192.

Cook, R., Dickinson, A., and Heyes, C. (2012). Contextual modulation of mirror and counter mirror sensorimotor associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 774–787.

Cook, R., Johnston, A., and Heyes, C. (2013). Facial self imitation: Objective measurement reveals no improvement without visual feedback. Psychological Science, 24(1), 93–98.

Cook, R., Press, C., Dickinson, A., and Heyes, C. (2010). Is the acquisition of automatic imitation sensitive to sensorimotor contingency? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 840–852.

Cook, R. G., Brown, M. F., and Riley, D. A. (1985). Flexible memory processing by rats: Use of prospective and retrospective information in the radial maze.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 11(3),453–469.

Cooper, R. P., and Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584–1595.

Cooper, R. P., Cook, R., Dickinson, A., and Heyes, C. (2013). Associative (not Heb bian) learning and the mirror neuron system. Neuroscience Letters, 540, 28–36. Corbeill, A. (2004). Nature embodied: Gesture in Ancient Rome. Princeton, NJ:Princeton University Press.

Cosmides, L.,and Tooby,J.(1994).Beyond intuition and instinct blindness:Toward an evolutionary rigorous cognitive science. Cognition, 50, 41–77.

Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (1997). Evolutionary Psychology: A Primer.https://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html.

Coussi Korbel, S., and Fragaszy., D. M. (1995). On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1441–1453.

Cowie, F. (2016). Innateness and language. In E. N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/innateness language.

Crain, S., Goro, T., and Thornton, R. (2006). Language acquisition is language change. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35(1), 31–49.

Cronk, L. (1991). Human behavioral ecology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 20(1), 25–53.

Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., and Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation denovo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuro Image, 31(3),1257–1267.

Csibra, G., and Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata and M. H. Johnson (eds.), Processes of Change in Brain and Cognitive Development (Vol. 21, Attention and Performance). Ox ford: Oxford University Press, 249–274.

Culicover, P. W. (1999). Syntactic Nuts: Hard Cases, Syntactic Theory, and Lan guage Acquisition (Vol. 1). Oxford University Press on Demand.

Culicover, P. W., and Jackendof, R. (2005). Simpler Syntax. Oxford: Oxford Uni versity Press.

Dąbrowska, E. (2012). Diferent speakers, diferent grammars: Individual diferences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(3), 219–253.

Damuth, J., and Heisler, I. L. (1988). Alternative formulations of multilevel selection. Biology and Philosophy, 3(4), 407–430.

Darwin, C. (1868). The Variation in Animals and Plants under Domestication.London: John Murray.

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London:John Murray.

Dawkins, R. (1989). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dawson, E. H., Avarguès Weber, A., Chittka, L., and Leadbeater, E. (2013).Learning by observation emerges from simple associations in an insect model. Current Biology, 23(8), 727–730.

De Casper, A. J., and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Newborn preference for the maternal voice: An indication of early attachment. Paper delivered at a meeting of the Southeastern Conference on Human Development, Alex andria, VA.

De Casper, A. J., and Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development,9(2), 133–150.

Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the Brain: The New Science of How We Read.London: Penguin.

Dehaene, S. (2014). Reading in the brain revised and extended: Response to comments. Mind and Language, 29(3), 320–335.

Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 254–262.

Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A.,Dehaene Lambertz, G., Kolinsky, R., Morais, J., and Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330(6009), 1359–1364.

De Klerk, C. C., Johnson, M. H., Heyes, C., and Southgate, V. (2015). Baby steps:Investigating the development of perceptual motor couplings in infancy. De velopmental Science, 18(2), 270–280.

Della Libera, C., and Chelazzi, L. (2006). Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards. Psychological Science, 17(3), 222–227.

Demetras, M. J., Post, K. N., and Snow, C. E. (1986). Feedback to first language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language, 13(2), 275–292.

Dennett, D. C. (1969). Content and Consciousness. London: Routledge.

Dennett, D. C. (1984). Cognitive wheels: The frame problem of AI. In C. Hookway(ed.), Minds, Machines and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 129–151.

Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Dennett, D. C. (1990). Memes and the exploitation of imagination. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 48(2), 127–135.

Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.

Dennett, D. C. (2001). The evolution of culture. The Monist, 84(3), 305–324.

Den Ouden, H. E., Friston, K. J., Daw, N. D., McIntosh, A. R., and Stephan, K. E.(2009). A dual role for prediction error in associative learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19(5), 1175–1185.

De Villiers, P. A., and de Villiers, J. G. (2012). Deception dissociates from false belief reasoning in deaf children: Implications for the implicit versus explicit theory of mind distinction. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 188–209.

Diaconescu, A. O., Mathys, C., Weber, L. A., Daunizeau, J., Kasper, L., Lomakina,E.I., ... and Stephan, K. E. (2014). Inferring on the intentions of others by hierarchical Bayesian learning. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(9), e1003810.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64,135–168.

Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. Cambridge: Cam bridge University Press.

Dickinson, A. (2012). Associative learning and animal cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 367(1603), 2733–2742.

Di Giorgio, E., Leo, I., Pascalis, O., and Simion, F. (2012). Is the face perception system human specific at birth?. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1083–1090.

Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180.

Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., and Liepelt,R. (2011). How “social” is the social Simon efect? Frontiers in Psychology.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00084.

Dorrance, B. R., and Zentall, T. R. (2001). Imitative learning in Japanese quail depends on the motivational state of the observer quail at the time of observation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 62–67.

Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., and Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–338.

Duncan, J., Schramm, M., Thompson, R., and Dumontheil, I. (2012). Task rules,working memory, and fluid intelligence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(5), 864–870.

Dupierrix, E., de Boisferon, A. H., Méary, D., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Di Giorgio,E., . . . and Pascalis, O. (2014). Preference for human eyes in human infants.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123, 138–146.

Duranti, A. (2008). Further reflections on reading other minds. Anthropological Quarterly, 81, 483–494.

Eco, U. (1995). The Search for the Perfect Language. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell. Efferson, C., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Edsten, E., Waring,T. M., Paciotti, B., and Baum, W. (2007). Learning, productivity, and noise:An experimental study of cultural transmission on the Bolivian Altiplano.Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 11–17.

Emberson, L. L., Richards, J. E., and Aslin, R. N. (2015). Top down modulation in the infant brain: Learning induced expectations rapidly afect the sensory cortex at 6 months. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(31),9585–9590.

Eriksson, K. (2012). The nonsense math efect. Judgment and Decision Making, 7, 746–749.

Evans, J. S. B., and Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3),223–241.

Evans, N. (2013). Multilingualism as the primal human condition: What we have to learn from small scale speech communities. Keynote address delivered at 9th International Symposium on Bilingualism, Singapore.

Evans, N., and Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(5), 429–448.

Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã.Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthro pology, 46, 621–646.

Eysenck, M. W., and Keane, M. T. (2015). Cognitive Psychology: A Student’s Hand book (7thed.). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press.

Fagot, J., and Cook, R. G. (2006). Evidence for large long term memory capacities in baboons and pigeons and its implications for learning and the evolution of cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,103(46), 17564–17567.

Falck Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., and von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9(7), 878–879.

Farroni, T., Mansfield, E. M., Lai, C., and Johnson, M. H. (2003). Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(3), 199–212.

Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., and Kanwisher, N. (2012). Language selective and domain general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Current Biology,22(21), 2059–2062.

Fernald, A. (1991). Prosody in speech to children: Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Annals of Child Development, 8, 43–80.

Fernandes, H. B., Woodley, M. A., and te Nijenhuis, J. (2014). Diferences in cognitive abilities among primates are concentrated on g: Phenotypic and phylogenetic comparisons with two meta analytical databases. Intelligence, 46, 311–322.

Ferrari, P. F., Rozzi, S., and Fogassi, L. (2005). Mirror neurons responding to observation of actions made with tools in monkey ventral premotor cortex.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 212–226.

Fiorito, G., and Scotto, P. (1992). Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris. Science, 256, 545–547.

Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., and Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition, 97, 179–210.

Flege, J. E., Yeni Komshian, G. H., and Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41(1), 78–104.

Fleming, S. M., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2012). Metacognition: Computation,biology and function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 367, 1280–1286.

Floccia, C., Christophe, A., and Bertoncini, J. (1997). High amplitude sucking and newborns: The quest for underlying mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(2), 175–198.

Fogarty, L., Rendell, L., and Laland, K. N. (2012). Mental time travel, memory and the social learning strategies tournament. Learning & Motivation, 43,241–246.

Forssberg, H,. Eliasson, A. C., Kinoshita, H., Johansson, R. S., and Westling, G.(1991). Developmentof human precision grip. I. Basic coordination of force.Experimental Brain Research, 85, 451–457.

Frank, M. C., Vul, E., and Johnson, S. P. (2009). Development of infants’ attention to faces during the first year. Cognition, 110(2), 160–170.

Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 287–313.

Frith, U. (2001). Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron, 32(6), 969–979. Fusaro, M., and Harris, P. L. (2008). Children assess in formant reliability using bystanders’ non verbal cues.Developmental Science, 11 (5), 771–777.

Gabi, M., Neves, K., Masseron, C., Ribeiro, P. F. M., Ventura Antunes, L., Torres, L.,Mota, B., Kaas, J. H., and Herculano Houzel, S. (2016). No relative expansion of the number of prefrontal neurons in primate and human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201610178.

Galef, B. G. (1971). Social efects in the weaning of domestic rat pups. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 75(3), 358–362.

Galef, B. G., Dudley, K. E., and Whiskin, E. E. (2008). Social learning of food preferences in “dissatisfied” and “uncertain” Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 75, 631–637.

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.

Gangestad, S. W. (2016). An evolutionary perspective on oxytocin and its behavioral efects. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 115–119.

Garcia, E., Baer, D. M., and Firestone, I. (1971). The development of generalized imitation within topographically determined boundaries. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 4(2), 101–112.

Garvert, M. M., Moutoussis, M., Kurth Nelson, Z., Behrens, T. E., and Dolan, R. J.(2015). Learning induced plasticity in medial prefrontal cortex predicts preference malleability. Neuron, 85(2), 418–428.

Giles, A., and Rovee Collier, C. (2011). Infant long term memory for associations formed during mere exposure. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(2),327–338.

Godfrey Smith, P. (2009). Darwinian Populations and Natural Selection. Oxford:Oxford University Press.

Godfrey Smith, P. (2012). Darwinism and cultural change. Philosophical Trans actions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599),2160–2170.

Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Goldstein, M. H., and Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants’ babbling facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psychological Science, 19(5), 515–523.

Goodnow, J. J. (1955). Determinants of choice distribution in two choice situations. The American Journal of Psychology, 68(1), 106–116.

Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085–1108.

Grafen, A. (1984). Natural selection, kin selection and group selection. In J. R.Krebs and N. B. Davies (eds.), Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach(2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific, 62–84.

Gray, R. D., and Atkinson, Q. D. (2003). Language tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo European origin. Nature, 426(6965), 435–439.

Greenwood, J. (2015). Becoming Human: The Ontogenesis, Metaphysics, and Ex pression of Human Emotionality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gregory, R., Cheng, H., Rupp, H. A., Sengelarb, D. R., and Heiman, J. R. (2015).Oxytocin increases VTA activation to infant and sexual stimuli in nulliparous and postpartum women. Hormones and Behavior, 69, 82–88.

Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66(3), 377–388.

Grifiths, P. E. (2017). The distinction between innate and acquired characteristics. In E. N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2017 Edition). https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/innate acquired/.

Grifiths, P. E., Pocheville, A., Calcott, B., Stotz, K., Kim, H., and Knight, R. (2015).Measuring causal specificity. Philosophy of Science, 82(4), 529–555.

Gruber, C. W. (2014). Physiology of invertebrate oxytocin and vasopressin neuropeptides. Experimental Physiology, 99, 51–55.

Grusec, J. E., and Abramovitch, R. (1982). Imitation of peers and adults in a natural setting: A functional analysis. Child Development, 53, 636–642.

Güss, C. D., and Wiley, B. (2007). Metacognition of problem solving strategies in Brazil, India, and the United States. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7,1–25.

Guzzon, D., Brignani, D., Miniussi, C., and Marzi, C. A. (2010). Orienting of attention with eye and arrow cues and the efect of over training. Acta Psycho logica, 134(3), 353–362.

Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E., and Wiley, E. (2003). Critical evidence: A test of the critical period hypothesis for second language acquisition. Psychological Science, 14(1), 31–38.

Harris, W. V. (ed.) (2013). Mental Disorders in the Classical World. Leiden: Brill. Harshaw, C., and Lickliter, R. (2007). Interactive and vicarious acquisition of auditory preferences in Northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus) chicks.Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(3), 320–331.

Harvey, O. J., and Rutherford, J. (1960). Status in the informal group: Influence and influencibility at difering age levels. Child Development, 31, 377–385.

Haslinger, B., Erhard, P., Altenmuller, E., Schroeder, U., Boecker, H., and Ceballos Baumann, A. O. (2005). Transmodal sensorimotor networks during action observation in professional pianists. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2),282–293.

Haun, D. B. M., Rapold, C. J., Call, J., Janzen, G., and Levinson, S. C. (2006). Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in Hominid spatial cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(46), 17568–17573.

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., and Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language:What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579.

Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D.R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., and Mat sumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self–improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599–615.

Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: The Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69, 197–214.

Henrich, J. (2015). The Secret of Our Success: How Culture is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton:Princeton University Press.

Henrich, J., and Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1139–1148.

Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., and Cable, C. (1976). Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2(4), 285–302.

Heyes, C. (1993). Imitation, culture and cognition. Animal Behaviour, 46(5),999–1010.

Heyes, C. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69(2), 207–231.

Heyes, C. (2003). Four routes of cognitive evolution. Psychological Review, 110(4), 713–727.

Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463–483.

Heyes, C. (2012a). Grist and mills: On the cultural origins of cultural learning.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences,367(1599), 2181–2191.

Heyes, C. (2012b). Simple minds: A qualified defence of associative learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 367(1603), 2695–2703.

Heyes, C. (2012c). What’s social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 193–202.

Heyes, C. (2013). What can imitation do for cooperation? In K. Sterelny, R. Joyce,B. Calcott, and B. Fraser (eds.), Cooperation and its Evolution. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press, 313–331.

Heyes, C. (2014a). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–143.

Heyes, C. (2014b). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17(5), 647–659.

Heyes, C. (2015). Animal mind reading: What’s the problem? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 313–327.

Heyes, C. (2016a). Blackboxing: Social learning strategies and cultural evolution.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences,371(1693), 20150369.

Heyes, C. (2016b). Born pupils? Natural pedagogy and cultural pedagogy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 280–295.

Heyes, C. (2016c). Who knows? Metacognitive social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(3), 204–213.

Heyes, C. (2016d). Imitation: Not in our genes. Current Biology, 26(10), R412–R414. Heyes, C. (2016e).

Homo imitans? Seven reasons why imitation couldn’t possibly be associative. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Bio logical Sciences, 371(1686), 20150069.

Heyes, C. (2017a). When does social learning become cultural learning? Developmental Science, 20(2), e12350.

Heyes, C. (2017b). Apes submentalise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(1), 1–2. Heyes, C. (in press, a). Enquire within: Cultural evolution and cognitive science. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences.

Heyes, C. (in press, b). Human nature, natural pedagogy, and evolutionary causal essentialism. In T. Lewens and E. Hannon (eds.), Why We Disagree about Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Heyes, C., Bird, G., Johnson, H., and Haggard, P. (2005). Experience modulates automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 233–240.

Heyes, C., and Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344(6190), 1243091.

Heyes, C., and Galef, Jr., B. G. (eds.) (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Heyes, C., and Pearce, J. M. (2015). Not so social learning strategies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 282(1802),20141709.

Heyes, C., and Ray, E. D. (2000). What is the significance of imitation in animals?Advances in the Study of Behavior, 29, 215–245.

Hill, M. R., Boorman, E. D., and Fried, I. (2016). Observational learning computations in neurons of the human anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Communications, 7, doi:10.1038/ncomms12722.

Holden, C., and Mace, R. (2009). Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults. Human Biology, 81(5/6), 597–619.

Holland, P. C. (1992). Occasion setting in Pavlovian conditioning. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 28, 69–125.

Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., and Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.

Hoppitt, W., and Laland, K. N. (2013). Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases afiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–960.

Hrdy, S. B. (2011). Mothers and Others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hsu, H. J., and Bishop, D. V. (2014). Sequence specific procedural learning deficits in children with specific language impairment. Developmental Science, 17(3), 352–365.

Hsu, H. J., Tomblin, J. B., and Christiansen, M. H. (2014). Impaired statistical learning of non adjacent dependencies in adolescents with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 175.

Hughes, C., Jafee, S. R., Happé, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A., and Mofitt, T. E. (2005).Origins of individual diferences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture?Child Development, 76(2), 356–370.

Hull, D. L. (1986). On human nature. In PSA: Proceedings of the biennial meeting of the philosophy of science association (Vol. 1986, No. 2), Baltimore, MD: Philosophy of Science Association, 3–13.

Hurks, P. P. M. (2012). Does instruction in semantic clustering and switching enhance verbal fluency in children? Clinical Neuropsychology, 26,1019–1037.

Insel, T. R., and Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(2), 129–136.

Ivanova, M. V., Isaev, D. Y., Dragoy, O. V., Akinina, Y. S., Petrushevskiy, A. G., Fe dina, O. N., . . . and Dronkers, N. F. (2016). Difusion tensor imaging of major white matter tracts and their role in language processing in aphasia. Cortex, 85, 165–181.

Jablonka, E., and Lamb, M. J. (2005): Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, Epigenetic, Behavioral, and Symbolic Variation in the History of Life. Cam bridge, MA: MIT Press.

Jackson, P. L., Meltzof, A. N., and Decety, J. (2006). Neural circuits involved in imitation and perspective taking.Neuro Image, 31(1), 429–439.

Jacobson, S. W., and Kagan, J. (1979). Interpreting “imitative” responses in early infancy. Science, 205, 215–217.

James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt.

Jelinek, E. (1995). Quantification in Straits Salish. In E. Bach, E. Jelinek, A.Kratzer, and B. Partee (eds.), Quantification in Natural Languages. Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer, 487–540.

Jensen, J., Smith, A. J., Willeit, M., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J., Vitcu, I., and Kapur, S. (2007). Separate brain regions code for salience versus valence during reward prediction in humans. Human Brain Mapping, 28,294–302.

Johnson, M. H. (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 766–774.

Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1), 1–19.

Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., and Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow?The elicitation of gaze following in 12 month olds. Developmental Science, 1(2), 233–238.

Jones, P. L., Ryan, M. J., Flores, V., and Page, R. A. (2013). When to approach novel prey cues? Social learning strategies in frog eating bats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 280(1772),20132330.

Jones, S. S. (2006). Exploration or imitation? The efect of music on 4 week old infants’ tongue protrusions. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 126–130.

Jones, S. S. (2007). Imitation in infancy: The development of mimicry. Psycho logical Science, 18, 593–599.

Jones, S. S. (2009). The development of imitation in infancy. Philosophical Trans actions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2325–2335.

Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). How infants begin to extract words from speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 323–328.

Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.

Kano, F., Call, J., and Tomonaga, M. (2012). Face and eye scanning in gorillas(Gorilla gorilla), orangutans (Pongo abelii), and humans (Homo sapiens) :Unique eye viewing patterns in humans among hominids. Journal of Com parative Psychology, 126(4), 388–398.

Karmilof Smith, A. (1995). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Karmilof Smith, A. (2015). An alternative to domain general or domain specific frameworks for theorizing about human evolution and ontogenesis. AIMS Neuroscience, 2(2), 91–104.

Katz, J. J. (1985). An outline of Platonist grammar. In Katz, J. J (ed.), The Philosophy of Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Brown, J., and Mackintosh, N. (2009).Associative learning predicts intelligence above and beyond working memory and processing speed. Intelligence, 37, 374–382.

Kaye, K., and Marcus, J. (1978). Imitation over a series of trials without feedback:Age six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 141–155.

Kepecs, A., and Mainen, Z. F. (2012). A computational framework for the study of confidence in humans and animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 367(1594), 1322–1337.

Keysers, C., Kohler, E., Umiltà, M. A., Nanetti, L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V.(2003). Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 153(4), 628–636.

Kidd, D. C., and Castano, E. (2013). Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science, 342(6156), 377–380.

Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48, 171–184.

Kidd, E., and Arciuli, J. (2016). Individual diferences in statistical learning predict children’s comprehension of syntax. Child Development, 87(1),184–193.

Kline, M. A., and Boyd, R. (2010). Population size predicts technological com plexity in Oceania. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Bio logical Sciences, 277(1693), 2559–2564.

Klopfer, P. H. (1961). Observational learning in birds: The establishment of behavioral modes. Behaviour, 17, 71–80.

Koepke, J. E., Hamm, M., Legerstee, M., and Russell, M. (1983). Neonatal imitation: Two failures to replicate. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 97–102.

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G.(2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297(5582), 846–848.

Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., and Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676.

Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., and Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012),1830–1834.

Kruglanski, A. W., and Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychological Review, 118(1), 97–109.

Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110–114.

Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10(1), 110–120.

Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Kuo, Z. Y. (1922). How are our instincts acquired? Psychological Review, 29(5),344–365.

Lakof, G. (1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image schemas? Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 39–74.

Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior, 32(1), 4–14.

Laland, K. N., and Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Laland, K. N., and Brown, G. R. (in press). The social construction of human nature. In T. Lewens and E. Hannon (eds.), Why We Disagree about Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Laland, K. N., and Rendell, L. (2013). Cultural memory. Current Biology, 23,R736–R740.

Lane, A., Mikolajczak, M., Treinen, E., Samson, D., Corneille, O., de Timary, P.,and Luminet, O. (2015). Failed replication of oxytocin efects on trust: The envelope task case. PloS One, 10(9), e0137000.

Lawson, E. A., Marengi, D. A., DeSanti, R. L., Holmes, T. M., Schoenfeld, D. A., and Tolley, C. J. (2015). Oxytocin decreases caloric intake in men. Obesity, 23,950–956.

Leadbeater, E. (2015). What evolves in the evolution of social learning? Journal of Zoology, 295(1), 4–11.

Lefebvre, L., and Giraldeau, L A. (1996). Is social learning an adaptive specialization? In C. Heyes and B. G. Galef (eds.), Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 107–152.

Leighton, J., Bird, G., and Heyes, C. (2010). “Goals” are not an integral component of imitation. Cognition, 114(3), 423–435.

Leighton, J., and Heyes, C. (2010). Hand to mouth: Automatic imitation across efector systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1174.

Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The Biological Foundations of Language. New York:Wiley.

Lepage, J. F., and Théoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: Grasping others’ actions from birth? Developmental Science, 10(5), 513–523.

Le Pelley, M. E. (2010). Attention and human associative learning. In C. J.Mitchell and M. E. Le Pelley (eds.), Attention and Associative Learning: From Brain to Behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press, 187–215.

Le Pelley, M. E., Vadillo, M., and Luque, D. (2013). Learned predictiveness influences rapid attentional capture: Evidence from the dot probe task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(6), 1888–1900.

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426.

Lewens, T. (2012). Human nature: The very idea. Philosophy & Technology, 25(4),459–474.

Lewens, T. (2015). Cultural Evolution: Conceptual Challenges. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lewens, T. and Hannon, E. (eds.) (in press). Why We Disagree about Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Kyriakidou, C., Maridaki Kassotaki, K., and Ber ridge, D. M. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67(6),2930–2947.

Li, J. (2003). US and Chinese cultural beliefs about learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 258–267.

Li, W., Howard, J. D., Parrish, T. B., and Gottfried, J. A. (2008). Aversive learning enhances perceptual and cortical discrimination of indiscriminable odor cues. Science, 319, 1842–1845.

Lidz, J., and Gagliardi, A. (2015). How nature meets nurture: Universal grammar and statistical learning. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 333–353.

Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289.

Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind.Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 3–32.

Lipton, P. (2003). Inference to the Best Explanation. London: Routledge.

Lohmann, H., and Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–1144.

Loman, M. M., and Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulationin children. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 867–876.

Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and the Modification of Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Lorenz, R., and Tinbergen, N. (1938/1970). Taxis and instinctive behavior pat tern in egg rolling by the Graylag goose. In K. Lorenz (ed.), R. Martin (trans.),Studies in Animal and in Human Behavior (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 316–359.

Lumsden, C. J., and Wilson, E. O. (2005). Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevo lutionary Process. Singapore: World Scientific.

Luna, B. (2009). Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Advances in Child Development & Behavior. 37, 233–278.

Lunt, L., Bramham, J., Morris, R. G., Bullock, P. R., Selway, R. P., Xenitidis, K., and David, A. S. (2012). Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and “jumping to conclusions”: Bias or deficit? Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(1), 65–78.

Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., and Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of over imitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50),19751–19756.

Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. London: Cambridge University Press.

Machery, E. (2008). A plea for human nature. Philosophical Psychology, 21(3),321–329.

Machery, E. (in press). A plea for human nature, redux. In T. Lewens and E. Hannon (eds.), Why We Disagree about Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Mack, A., and Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82(4), 276–298.

MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., Au reli, F., Baker, J. M., Bania, A. E., Barnard, A. M., and Boogert, N. J. (2014). The evolution of self control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(20), E2140–E2148.

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., and Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional dis orders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15–20.

MacPhail, E. M. (1982). Brain and Intelligence in Vertebrates. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Mahmoodi, A., Bang, D., Ahmadabadi, M. N., and Bahrami, B. (2013). Learning to make collective decisions: The impact of confidence escalation. PLoS One, 8, e81195.

Margulis, E. H., Mlsna, L. M., Uppunda, A. K., Parrish, T. B., and Wong, P. C.(2009). Selective neurophysiologic responses to music in instrumentalists with diferent listening biographies. Human Brain Mapping, 30(1),267–275.

Masters, J. C. (1979). Interpreting “imitative” responses in early infancy. Science, 205(4402), 215.

Matzel, L. D., and Kolata, S. (2010). Selective attention, working memory, and animal intelligence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(1), 23–30.

Mayer, A., and Träuble, B. E. (2013). Synchrony in the onset of mental state understanding across cultures? A study among children in Samoa. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37, 21–28.

Maynard Smith, J. (2000). The concept of information in biology. Philosophy of Science, 67, 177–194.

McBrearty, S., and Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: A new inter pretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Humantion, 39(5), 453–563.

McEwen, F., Happé, F., Bolton, P., Rijsdijk, F., Ronald, A., Dworzynski, K., and Plomin, R. (2007). Origins of individual diferences in imitation: Links with language, pretend play, and socially insightful behavior in two year old twins.Child Development, 78(2), 474–492.

McGeer, V. (2007). The regulative dimension of folk psychology. In D. D. Hutto and M. Ratclife (eds.), Folk Psychology Re-Assessed. New York: Springer,137–156.

McGuigan, N. (2013). The influence of model status on the tendency of young children to over imitate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4),962–969.

McKenzie, B., and Over, R. (1983). Young infants fail to imitate facial and manual gestures. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 85–95.

Meins, E. (2012). Social relationships and children’s understanding of mind:At tachment, internal states, and mind mindedness. In M. Siegal and L. Surian(eds.), Access to Language and Cognitive Development. Oxford: Oxford Uni versity Press, 23–43.

Mellars, P. (1989). Major issues in the emergence of modern humans. Current An thropology, 30(3), 348–385.

Mellars, P. (2005). The impossible coincidence. A single species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14,12–27.

Mellars, P., and Stringer, C. (eds.) (1989). The Human Revolution. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Meltzof, A. N. (1988). The human infant as Homo imitans. In T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef (eds.), Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 319–341.

Meltzof, A. N. (2002). Imitation as a mechanism of social cognition: Origins of empathy, theory of mind, and the representation of action. In U. Goswami(ed.), Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Oxford:Blackwell, 6–25.

Meltzof, A. N. (2005). Imitation and other minds: The “like me” hypothesis. In S. Hurley and N. Chater (eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 55–78.

Meltzof, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.

Meltzof, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1979). Interpreting “imitative” responses in early infancy. Science, 205, 217–219.

Meltzof, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior & Development, 17, 83–99.

Meltzof, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development & Parenting, 6(3–4), 179–192.

Mengotti, P., Ticini, L. F., Waszak, F., Schutz Bosbach, S., and Rumiati, R. I. (2013).Imitating others’ actions: Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal opercula reveals the processes underlying automatic imitation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 37(2), 316–322.

Meristo, M., Hjelmquist, E., and Morgan, G. (2012). How access to language affects theory of mind in deaf children. In M. Siegal and L. Surian (eds.), Access to Language and Cognitive Development. New York: Oxford University Press, 44–61.

Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory Can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Murray, K., and Jing Lu, H. (2015). Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the West shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,Series B: Biological Sciences,282, 20142209.

Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., and Laland, K. N. (2004). Perspective:Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of The Origin of Species. Evolution, 58(1), 1–11.

Michel, G. F., and Tyler, A. N. (2005). Critical period: A history of the transition from questions of when, to what, to how. Developmental Psychobiology, 46(3), 156–162.

Millikan, R. G. (1984). Language, Thought, and Other Biological Categories: New Foundations for Realism. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Misyak, J. B., and Christiansen, M. H. (2012). Statistical learning and language:An individual diferences study. Language Learning, 62(1), 302–331.

Moerk, E. L. (1991). Positive evidence for negative evidence. First Language, 11(32), 219–251.

Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., and Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(1), 341–349.

Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., and Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two day olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 495–500.

Moore, C., and Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14(4), 349–372.

Moore, J. W., Dickinson, A., and Fletcher, P. C. (2011). Sense of agency, associative learning, and schizotypy. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 792–800.

Moore, R. (2016). Gricean communication and cognitive development. The Philosophical Quarterly, 67(267), 303–326.

Moore, R. (in press). Social cognition, stag hunts, and the evolution of language. Biology & Philosophy.

Morgan, T. J. H., Rendell, L. E., Ehn, M., Hoppitt, W., and Laland, K. N. (2012).The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 279(1729), 653–662.

Morin, O. (2015). How Traditions Live and Die. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Muthukrishna, M., and Henrich, J. (2016). Innovation in the collective brain.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 371(1690), 20150192.

Nicolle, A., Klein Flügge, M. C., Hunt, L. T., Vlaev, I., Dolan, R. J., and Behrens,T. E. (2012). An agentin dependent axis for executed and modeled choice in medial prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 75(6), 1114–1121.

Nile, E., and Van Bergen, P. (2015). Not all semantics: Similarities and diferences in reminiscing function and content between indigenous and non indigenous Australians. Memory, 23(1), 83–98.

Nisbett, R. (2010). The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Diferently . . . and Why. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Norman, D. A., and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. J. Davidson, et al. (eds.), Consciousness and Self regulation (Vol. 4). Dordrecht, NL: Plenum, 1–18.

O’Brien, K., Slaughter, V., and Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sibling influences on theory of mind development for children with ASD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(6), 713–719.

Okasha, S. (2005). Multilevel selection and the major transitions in evolution.Philosophy of Science, 72(5), 1013–1025.

Olsson, A., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1095–1102.

Onishi, K. H., and Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15 month old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.

Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy Costantini, S.,Davis, J., . . . and Slaughter, V. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26(10), 1334–1338.

Osada, T. (1992). A Reference Grammar of Mundari. Institute for the Study of Languages and Cultures of Asia and Africa, Tokyo University of Foreign Studies.

O’Toole, R., and Dubin, R. (1968). Baby feeding and body sway: An experiment in George Herbert Mead’s “Taking the role of the other.” Journal of Person ality and Social Psychology, 10, 59–65.

Ozturk, O.,Krehm,M.,and Vouloumanos,A.(2013).Sound symbolism in infancy: Evidence for sound shape cross modal correspondences in 4 month olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 173–186.

Pagel, M. (2000). The history, rate and pattern of world linguistic evolution. In C.Knight, M. Studdert Kennedy, and J. Hurford (eds.), The Evolutionary Emergence of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 391–416.

Papineau, D. (1987). Reality and Representation. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Paracchini, S., Scerri, T., and Monaco, A. P. (2007). The genetic lexicon of dyslexia. Annual Review of Genomics, Human Genetics, 8, 57–79.

Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., and Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species specific during the first year of life? Science, 296(5571), 1321–1323.

Passingham, R. E. (2008). What is Special about the Human Brain? New York:Oxford University Press.

Passingham, R. E., and Smaers, J. B. (2014). Is the prefrontal cortex especially enlarged in the human brain? Allometric relations and remapping factors.Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 84(2), 156–166.

Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Vissers, M., and Bekkering, H. (2011). Imitation in in fancy: Rational or motor resonance? Child Development, 82(4), 1047–1057.

Pawlby, S. J. (1977). Imitative interaction. In H. Schafer (ed.), Studies in Mother–Infant Interaction. New York: Academic Press, 203–224.

Pearce, J. M. (2013). Animal Learning and Cognition: An Introduction. London:Psychology Press.

Perner, J. (2010). Who took the cog out of cognitive science? In P. A. Frensch and R. Schwarzer (eds.), Cognition and Neuropsychology: International Perspectives on Psychological Science (Vol. 1). London: Psychology Press,241–261.

Petersen, S. E., and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89.

Piaget, J. (1962). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. New York: Norton. Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct: The New Science of Language and Mind.London: Penguin.

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. London: Penguin.

Pinker, S., and Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–727.

Pinker, S., and Jackendof, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 95(2), 201–236.

Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., and McGufin, P. (2001). Behavior Genetics. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Plotkin, H. C., and Odling Smee, F. J. (1981). A multiple level model of evolution and its implications for sociobiology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(2),225–235.

Pocheville, A., and Danchin, E. (2017). Genetic assimilation and the paradox of blind variation. In P. Huneman and D. Walsh (eds.), Challenging the Modern Synthesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59–63.

Popper, K. (1962). Conjectures and Refutations. New York: Basic Books.

Prados, J. (2011). Blocking and overshadowing in human geometry learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 37, 121–126.

Press, C., Bird, G., Flach, R., and Heyes, C. (2005). Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 632–640.

Pyers, J. E., and Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false belief under standing: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science, 20(7), 805–812.

Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., and Fried, I. (2005). Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature, 435(7045), 1102–1110.

Qureshi, A. W., Apperly, I. A., and Samson, D. (2010). Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level1 visual perspective calculation:Evidence from a dual task study of adults.Cognition, 117(2), 230–236.

Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo,J., Coppola, G., Geyer, M. A., Glanzman, D. L., Marsland, S., and McSweeney,F. K. (2009). Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 135–138.

Rashotte, M. E., Grifin, R. W., and Sisk, C. L. (1977). Second order conditioning of the pigeon’s keypeck. Learning & Behavior, 5(1), 25–38.

Ray, E., and Heyes, C. (2011). Imitation in infancy: The wealth of the stimulus.Developmental Science, 14(1), 92–105.

Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., and Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 366, 1017–1027.

Reader, S. M., and Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 4436–4441.

Reeb Sutherland, B. C., Fifer, W. P., Byrd, D. L., Hammock, E. A., Levitt, P., and Fox, N. A. (2011). One month old human infants learn about the social world while they sleep. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1134–1141.

Reeb Sutherland, B. C., Levitt, P., and Fox, N. A. (2012). The predictive nature of individual diferences in early associative learning and emerging social be havior. PloS One, 7(1), e30511.

Reid, V. M., Dunn, K., Young, R. J., Amu, J., Donovan, T., and Reissland, N. (2017).The human fetus preferentially engages with face like visual stimuli.Current Biology, 27, 1825–1828.

Reimers Kipping, S., Hevers, W., Pääbo, S., and Enard, W. (2011). Humanized FOXP2 specifically affects cortico basal ganglia circuits. Neuroscience, 175, 75–84.

Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W. J., Morgan, T. J., Webster, M. M., and Laland,K. N. (2011). Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(2), 68–76.

Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 43(3), 151–160.

Rescorla, R. A., and Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning:Variations in the efectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, 2, 64–99.

Richerson, P. J. (in press). What work (or mischief ) does“ human nature” do in the work of scientists? In T. Lewens and E. Hannon (eds.), Why We Disagree about Human Nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (2013). Rethinking paleoanthropology: A world queerer than we supposed. In G. Hatfield and H. Pittman (eds.), Evolution of Mind, Brain, and Culture. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press,263–302.

Rilling, J. K. (2014). Comparative primate neuroimaging: Insights into human brain evolution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 46–55.

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Re view of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

Rogers, A. R. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.

Rosa Salva, O., Regolin, L., and Vallortigara, G. (2010). Faces are special for newly hatched chicks: Evidence for inborn domain specific mechanisms under lying spontaneous preferences for face like stimuli. Developmental Science, 13(4), 565–577.

Rovee Collier, C., and Giles, A. (2010). Why a neuromaturational model of memory fails: Exuberant learning in early infancy. Behavioural Processes, 83(2), 197–206.

Rovee Collier, C., Mitchell, K., and Hsu Yang, V. (2013). Efortlessly strengthening infant memory: Associative potentiation of new learning. Scandina vian Journal of Psychology, 54(1), 4–9.

Rushworth, M. F., Mars, R. B., and Sallet, J. (2013). Are there specialized circuits for social cognition, and are they uniqueto humans? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 436–442.

Ryle, G. (1945). “Philosophical Arguments.” Inaugural lecture, Waynflete Professor of Metaphysical Philosophy. Reprinted (2009) in Collected Papers of Gilbert Ryle, Vol. 2. London: Routledge, 194–211.

Saggerson, A. L., George, D. N., and Honey, R. C. (2005). Imitative learning of stimulus response and response outcome associations in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 289–300.

Salmon, K., and Reese, E. (2016). The benefits of reminiscing with young children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 233–238.

Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., and Bodley Scott, S. E.(2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5), 1255.

Samuels, R. (2004). Innateness in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 136–141.

Samuels, R. (2012). Science and human nature. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement, 70, 1–28.

Santiesteban, I., Catmur, C., Hopkins, S. C., Bird, G., and Heyes, C. (2014). Avatars and arrows: Implicit mentalizing or domain general processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(3), 929–937.

Scheele, D., Wille, A., Kendrick, K. M., Stofel Wagner, B., Becker, B., Gunturkun,O., Maier, W., and Hurlemann, R. (2013). Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses of men viewing the face of their female partner. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 20308–20313.

Schreiweis, C., Bornschein, U., Burguière, E., Kerimoglu, C., Schreiter, S.,Dannemann, M., Goyal, S., Rea, E., French, C. A., Puliyadi, R., and Groszer, M.(2014). Humanized FOXP2 accelerates learning by enhancing transitions from declarative to procedural performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(39), 14253–14258.

Schultz, W., and Dickinson, A. (2000). Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 473–500.

Senju, A., and Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18(9), 668–671.

Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., and Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885.

Senzaki, S., Masuda, T., and Ishii, K. (2014). When is perception top down and when is it not? Culture, narrative, and attention. Cognitive Science, 38(7),1493–1506.

Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., and Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1239–1247.

Shanks, D. R. (2010). Learning: from association to cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 273–301.

Shannon, C. E. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. In C. E.Shannon and W. Weaver (eds.), The Mathematical Theory of Communication.Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

Shea, N. (2009). Imitation as an inheritance system. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2429–2443.

Shea, N. (2013). Inherited representations are read in development. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 64(1), 1–31.

Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., and Frith, C. D. (2014). Supra personal cognitive control and meta cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 186–193.

Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Shiraev, E., and Levy, D. A. (2014). Cross-Cultural Psychology. New York: Pearson Education Limited.

Shweder, R. A., and Sullivan, M. A. (1993). Cultural psychology: Who needs it?Annual Review of Psychology, 44(1), 497–523.

Silk, J. B., and House, B. R. (2011). Evolutionary foundations of human prosocial sentiments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(2),10910–10917.

Simons, D. J., and Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained in attentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074.

Siqueland, E. R., and Lipsitt, L. P. (1966). Conditioned head turning inhuman newborns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3(4), 356–376.

Skerry, A. E., Carey, S. E., and Spelke, E. S. (2013). First person action experience reveals sensitivity to action eficiency in prereaching infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(46), 18728–18733.

Skuse, D. H. (1993). Extreme deprivation in early childhood. In D. Bishop and K. Mogford (eds.), Language Development in Exceptional Circumstances.Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 29–46.

Skuse, D. H., and Gallagher, L. (2011). Genetic influences on social cognition. Pediatric Research, 69, 85R–91R.

Slaughter, V., and Peterson, C. C. (2012). How conversational input shapes theory of mind development in infancy and early childhood. In M.

Siegal and L. Surian (eds.), Access to Language and Cognitive Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 3–22.

Smith, L. B., Suanda, S. H., and Yu, C. (2014). The unrealized promise of infant statistical word–referent learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5),251–258.

Smith, N. A., and Trainor, L. J. (2008). Infant directed speech is modulated by infant feedback. Infancy, 13(4), 410–420.

Snowdon, C. T. (2004). Social processes in the evolution of complex cognition and communication. In D. Kimbrough Oller and U. Griebel (eds.), Evolution of Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 131–150.

Sober, E. (1991). Models of cultural evolution. In P. Grifiths (ed.), Trees of Life: Essays in the Philosophy of Biology. Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Southgate, V., Senju, A., and Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2 year olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587–592.

Spelke, E. S., and Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 89–96.

Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

Sperber, D. (2000a). An objection to the memetic approach to culture. In R. Aunger (ed.), Darwinizing Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Science.Oxford: Oxford University Press, 163–174.

Sperber, D. (2000b). Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (ed.), Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Oxford:Oxford University Press, 117–137.

Sperber, D., and Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition.2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a Hostile World: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

Sterelny, K. (2006). Memes revisited. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 57(1), 145–165.

Sterelny, K. (2012). The Evolved Apprentice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sterelny, K. (2017). Cultural evolution in California and Paris. Studies in the History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 62,42–50.

Sterelny, K. (in press). Culture and the extended phenotype: Cognition and material culture in deep time. In A. Newen, L. de Bruin, and S. Gallagher(eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognition: Embodied, Embedded, Enactive and Extended. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sterelny, K., Smith, K. C., and Dickison, M. (1996). The extended replicator. Biology and Philosophy, 11(3), 377–403.

Street, J. A., and Dąbrowska, E. (2010). More individual diferences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers? Lingua, 120(8), 2080–2094.

Tamariz, M., and Kirby, S. (2016). The cultural evolution of language. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 37–43.

Tamir, M., and Robinson, M. D. (2007). The happy spotlight: Positive mood and selective attention to rewarding information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(8), 1124–1136.

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., and Dunbar, R. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biology Letters, 11(10), 20150767.

Taumoepeau, M. (2016). Maternal expansions of child language relate to growth in children’s vocabulary. Language Learning and Development, 12(4), 429–446.

Taumoepeau, M., and Rufman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77(2), 465–481.

Taumoepeau, M., and Rufman, T. (2008). Stepping stones to others’ minds:Maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion under standing at 15, 24, and 33 months. Child Development, 79, 284–302.

Templeton, J. J., Kamil, A. C., and Balda, R. P. (1999). Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: The Pinyon Jay and the Clark’s Nutcracker. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 450–455.

Ten Cate, C., and Okanoya, K. (2012). Revisiting the syntactic abilities of non human animals: Natural vocalizations and artificial grammar learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1984–1994.

Tennie, C., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2405–2415.

Thornton, A., and McAulife, K. (2012). Teaching can teach us a lot. Animal Behaviour, 83(4), e6–e9.

Thornton, A., and Raihani, N. J. (2008). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75(6), 1823–1836.

Thurman, S. M., and Lu, H. (2013). Physical and biological constraints govern perceived animacy of scrambled human forms. Psychological Science, 24,1133–1141.

Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsy chologie, 20(4), 410–433.

Tipples, J. (2008). Orienting to counter predictive gaze and arrow cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(1), 77–87.

Toelch, U., Bruce, M., Newson, L., Richerson, P. J., and Reader, S. M. (2014).Individual consistency and flexibility in human social information use.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 281,20132864.

Tomasello, M. (1995). Language is not an instinct. Cognitive Development, 10,131–156.

Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language. A Usage-Based Approach. Cam bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M. (2009). The Cultural Originsof Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA:Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M. (2014). A Natural Historyof Human Thinking. Cambridge, MA:Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. (2005). Under standing and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

Tomasello M., Kruger A. C., and Ratner H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.

Tomasello, M., and Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intention ality and culture. In P. Kapperler and J. Silk (eds.), Mind the Gap. Tracing the Originsof Human Universals. Berlin: Springer, 331–349.

Tomblin, J. B., Mainela Arnold, E., and Zhang, X. (2007). Procedural learning in adolescents with and without specific language impairment. Language Learning and Development, 3(4), 269–293.

Tomblin, J. B., Shriberg, L., Murray, J., Patil, S., and Williams, C. (2004). Speech and language characteristics associated with a 7/13 translocation involving FOXP2. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 130, 97–97.

Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., and Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11(3), 188–195.

Trainor, L. J., and Desjardins, R. N. (2002). Pitch characteristics of infant directed speech afect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 335–340.

Triesch, J., Teuscher, C., Deák, G. O., and Carlson, E. (2006). Gaze following: Why(not) learn it? Developmental Science, 9(2), 125–147.

Tunçgenç, B., and Cohen, E. (2016). Movement synchrony forges social bonds across group divides. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 782.

Uller, T. (2008). Developmental plasticity and the evolution of parental efects.Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 23, 432–438.

Uzgiris, I. C. (1972). Patterns of vocal and gestural imitation in infants. In F. Monks, W. Hartup, and J. de Witt (eds.), Determinants of Behavioral De velopment. New York: Academic Press, 467–471.

Uzgiris, I. C., Benson, J. B., Kruper, J. C., and Vasek, M. E. (1989). Contextual influences on imitative interactions between mothers and infants. In J. Lockman and N. Hazen (eds.), Action in Social Context: Perspectives on Early Development. New York: Plenum Press, 103–127.

Van de Waal, E., Renevey, N., Farve, C. M., and Bahary, R. (2010). Selective attention to philopatric models causes directed social learning in wild vervet monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20092260.

Van derLely, H. K., and Pinker, S. (2014). The biological basis of language: In sight from developmental grammatical impairments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(11), 586–595.

Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 829–858.

Van Schaik, C. P., and Pradhan, G. R. (2003). A model for tool use traditions in primates: Implications for the coevolution of culture and cognition. Journal of Human Evolution, 44, 645–664.

Vecera, S. P., and Johnson, M. H. (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition, 2(1),59–87.

Vetter, N. C., Leipold, K., Kliegel, M., Phillips, L. H., and Altgassen, M. (2013). On going development of social cognition in adolescence. Child Neuropsychology, 19(6), 615–629.

Voelkl, B., and Huber, L. (2007). Imitation as faithful copying of a novel technique in marmoset monkeys. PLoS One, 2(7), e611.

Vogt, S., Buccino, G., Wohlschlager, A. M., Canessa, N., Shah, N. J., Zilles, K.,Eickhof, S. B., Freund, H. J., Rizzolatti, G., and Fink, G. R. (2007). Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Efects of practice and expertise. Neuro Image, 37(4), 1371–1383.

Vouloumanos, A., Hauser, M. D., Werker, J. F., and Martin, A. (2010). The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child Development, 81(2), 517–527.

Vouloumanos, A., and Werker, J. F. (2007). Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science, 10(2),159–164.

Washburn, M. F. (1908). The Animal Mind. New York: Macmillan.

Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism. Chicago: Phoenix.

Watson, J. S., and Ramey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 18(3), 219–227.

Webster, M. M., and Laland, K. N. (2015). Space use and sociability are not related to public information use in ninespine sticklebacks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69(6), 895–907.

Werker, J. F., and Hensch, T. K. (2015). Critical periods in speech perception: New directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 173–196.

Werner, C., and Latane, B. (1974). Interaction motivates attraction: Rats are fond of fondling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29(3), 328–334.

West Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford:Oxford University Press.

West Eberhard, M. J. (2005). Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(1),6543–6549.

White, B. L., Castle, P., and Held, R. (1964). Observations on the development of visually guided reaching. Child Development, 35, 349–364.

Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of Religiosity: A Cognitive Theory of Religious Transmission. Lanham, MA: Rowman Altamira.

Whiten, A.,and Custance,D.(1996).Studies of imitation in chimpanzees and children. In C. Heyes and B. G. Galef (eds.), Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture . San Diego: Academic Press, 291–318.

Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y.,Tutin, C. E., Wrangham, R. W., and Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpan zees. Nature, 399, 682–685.

Whiten, A., and Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 21, 239–283.

Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall Pescini, S., and Hopper, L. M. (2009).Emulation, imitation, over imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2417–2428.

Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., and Fitch, W. T. (2014). The “domestication syndrome” in mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell be havior and genetics. Genetics, 197(3), 795–808.

Wilkinson, A., Kuenstner, K., Mueller, J., and Huber, L. (2010). Social learning in a non social reptile. Biology Letters, 6, 614–616.

Williamson, R. A., Meltzof, A. N., and Markman, E. M. (2008). Prior experiences and perceived eficacy influence 3 year olds’ imitation.Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 275–285.

Wilson, B., Slater, H., Kikuchi, Y., Milne, A. E., Marslen Wilson, W. D., Smith, K.,and Petkov, C. I. (2013). Auditory artificial grammar learning in macaque and marmoset monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(48), 18825–18835.

Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wiltermuth, S. S., and Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5.

Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R., and Boroditsky, L.(2007). Russian blues reveal efects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(19), 7780–7785.

Wisdom, T. N., Song, X., and Goldstone, R. L. (2013). Social learning strategies in networked groups. Cognitive Science 37, 1383–1425.

Wobber, V., Hare, B., Lipson, S., Wrangham, R., and Ellison, P. (2013). Diferent ontogenetic patterns of testosterone production reflect divergent male re productive strategies in chimpanzees and bonobos. Physiology & Behavior, 116, 44–53.

Woodward, J. (2010). Causation in biology: Stability, specificity, and the choice of levels of explanation. Biology & Philosophy, 25(3), 287–318.

Wunderlich, K., Symmonds, M., Bossaerts, P., and Dolan, R. J. (2011). Hedging your bets by learning reward correlations in the human brain. Neuron, 71,1141–1152.

Yantis, S., and Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention:Voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 121–134.

Yong, E. (2012). Dark side of love. New Scientist, 213, 39–41.

Young, J. M., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., and Poulson, C. L. (1994). Generalized imitation and response class formation in children with autism.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(4), 685–697.

Zentall, T. R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9(4), 335–353.

Zilles, K. (2005). Evolution of the human brain and comparative cytoandreceptor architecture. In S. Dehaene, J. R. Duhamel, M. D. Hauser, and G. Riz zolatti (eds.), From Monkey Brain to Human Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 41–56.

Zuhurudeen, F. M., and Huang, Y. T. (2016). Efects of statistical learning on the acquisition of grammatical categories through Qur’anic memorization: A natural experiment. Cognition, 148, 79–84.

致谢

我不仅要感谢影响了本书核心思想的那些人,也要感谢对我的早期思想产生深远影响的那些人,是他们塑造了这个独特头脑的“谷”和“磨”。其中最重要的是我的哥哥文森特·海耶斯,他教会我如何辩论,以及如何正确地享受这件事胜过其他几乎所有事情。后来,我学术道路上的引导人是我的博士生导师亨利·普罗特金和博士后顾问唐纳德·坎贝尔、丹尼尔·丹尼特、安东尼·迪金森和尼古拉斯·麦金托什。回想起来,我很高兴他们之间有如此深刻的知识上的分歧,因为从进化认识论的高层跳入关联性学习的知识领域并不总是一帆风顺的。在美国,我和坎贝尔、丹尼特一起待了两年,之后,当我来到了剑桥的迪金森-麦金托什实验室时,我确信自己将坚定不移地转向,拥抱经验主义,放弃大自然和它的所有作品。但我的顾问们的远见卓识和作为知识分子的正直都鼓励着我去寻找正确的方法。本书涵盖了我目前为止在学术各方面取得的进展。

在那些塑造了这本书的“谷”的人中,我特别感谢那些有高贵灵魂的敏锐头脑,他们阅读了完整的书稿,并给出了宝贵的反馈:马丁·艾默、克里斯·弗里斯、尼克·谢伊、金·斯特莱尼,以及由雷切尔·布朗和理查德·摩尔在澳大利亚国立大学哲学系召集的每周一次的讨论小组的成员。(可惜的是,我不能参加讨论组,但我知道讨论常常进行得非常激烈。这使我觉得,如金·斯特莱尼所言,这是一个好兆头。)这些慷慨的第一读者——与艾伦·克拉克、安东尼·狄金森、尤塔·弗里斯和约翰·皮尔斯一起——不仅在书的主题上对我进行了辅导和挑战,而且在我从“实验心理学家”过渡到“理论生命科学家”时提供了宝贵的道义、实践和智力支持。

在伦敦大学学院和牛津大学之间的那条路上,我虽然放弃了实验室,但并没有忘记实验室生活的要求。我需要写更多的文章,筹集更多的资金,并让斤斤计较的资助方满意,还承担教学和行政工作,我很难找到时间读书,这也是为什么我花了很长时间努力使这本书尽可能简短精练。它是为了方便所有对人类进化感兴趣的人(包括人类学家、考古学家、生物学家、经济学家、哲学家和非专业读者)而写的,并且足够短,以便那些很少读心理学和神经科学领域书籍的人能迅速接受。

一旦我开始忘记实验室生活的严酷,杰弗·伯德、卡罗琳·卡特穆尔、理查德·库克、简·莱顿、克莱尔·普雷斯和伊丽莎白·雷会“粗暴地”提醒我。我和这些朋友和同事一起研究出了第6章中的模仿理论,他们不断提醒我设计和完成一个好的实验是多么困难,当你和你崇拜的人一起工作时,一切又是多么值得。

2011年,罗伯特·巴顿、伊娃·贾布隆卡、艾莉森·戈普尼克、拉塞尔·格雷、亚瑟·罗布森和金·斯特莱尼来到牛津大学,就人类认知的进化问题举办了一个为期一学期的研讨会。我感谢他们当时的赞助公司,感谢英格兰的亨利六世、亨利·奇切利大主教,以及牛津大学万灵学院的院长和同事们,他们主办了这次研讨会,并提供了一个理想的学术环境,让我们可以自由地讨论几乎所有事情。

这是我的第一本,也可能是唯一的一本书。我想感谢斯蒂芬·科利尼、塞西尔·法布尔和安东尼·戈特利布对本书写作的指导与建议,以及哈佛大学出版社的工作人员贾尼斯·奥黛特、埃默雷德·詹森-罗伯茨、伊恩·马尔科姆和斯蒂芬妮·维斯对所有细枝末节的耐心帮助。

我的搭档马丁·艾默,在他的诸多天赋中,非凡的地点记忆力令人印象深刻。每当我们的讨论回到本书中的某一个主题时,马丁就能回忆起我们上次解决这个问题的地方——例如,在海格特伍兹散步时我们讨论的电子语义,在热那亚一家咖啡馆外我们解决了内隐心智解读,在去朋友婚礼途中我们讨论着语言的普遍性——并且通过这种方法回忆起我们所说的几乎每一句话。除了这种天赋,更多的是对他在这些未被遗忘的讨论中给予的敏锐见解和热情鼓励,我深表感激。


小结这是最后一篇